ARE YOU SURE YOU'RE RIGHT?
Part 2
Part 2
By: Sora Bradshaw
Biases in Lineup Administration
Biases in lineup administration further relate to the reliability and fairness of eyewitness identifications. One critical source of bias is the influence of non-verbal or verbal cues from lineup administrators. These subtle cues, such as changes in tone, body language, or facial expressions, can inadvertently guide witnesses toward selecting a particular individual. Research underscores the danger of such influences, as they can lead to wrongful identifications and undermine the objectivity of the procedure.
To counteract these effects, double-blind administration has become a gold standard in lineup procedures. In a double-blind setup, neither the witness nor the lineup administrator knows the identity of the suspect. This arrangement eliminates the potential for administrator influence, ensuring that witness decisions are driven solely by their memory. As Greenspan and Loftus (2023) highlight, "double-blind procedures align with the core psychological principle of minimizing external influences on human decision-making," making them a crucial reform in pursuit of accuracy and fairness.
Cultural differences add another layer of complexity to lineup procedures. Variations in cultural values can influence how witnesses approach the identification task. For example, research indicates that individuals from individualistic cultures, such as those in Western societies, tend to emphasize personal confidence and assertiveness in their choices. This emphasis on confidence may cause witnesses to exhibit overconfidence in their selections, even when they are uncertain. By contrast, collectivistic cultures, often found in East Asian and some Latin American societies, prioritize social harmony and conformity. Witnesses from these cultures may avoid challenging the perceived expectations of the lineup process, leading to selection choices that aim to maintain group harmony rather than prioritize accuracy (Wang, 2021). These cultural factors highlight the need for culturally sensitive lineup procedures that recognize and address the influence of sociocultural norms on eyewitness behavior.
Addressing these biases requires the implementation of systemic reforms, such as double-blind administration, cultural competence training for law enforcement, and the development of culturally appropriate lineup protocols. Together, these measures aim to enhance the reliability of eyewitness identifications.
Technological Innovations in Lineup Procedures
AI-assisted lineup systems offer significant opportunities to reduce human bias and improve the fairness of eyewitness identifications. By using algorithms to ensure lineup members are similar in appearance to the suspect, AI reduces the likelihood that any one individual stands out, thereby minimizing the influence of suggestive lineup compositions. This approach addresses a well-documented problem in traditional lineups, where lineup fillers often differ noticeably from the suspect, inadvertently drawing the witness's attention.
However, the use of AI in lineup administration also raises important ethical and psychological questions. One concern is the potential for over-reliance on AI, where law enforcement and legal professionals may place undue trust in the technology's output, overlooking its limitations. AI systems are only as unbiased as the data and algorithms that shape them. If the dataset used to train the AI contains implicit racial or demographic biases, those biases could be perpetuated in lineup construction. Additionally, the psychological impact of AI-assisted lineups on witnesses warrants further investigation. Witnesses may perceive AI-generated lineups as more "objective" or "authoritative," which could increase their confidence in their identification, even when their memory is uncertain (Quigley-McBride & Negrabee, 2024). This overconfidence could have serious implications for the accuracy of identifications and the potential for wrongful convictions.
Despite these challenges, AI offers the potential to create more standardized and unbiased lineup procedures. Key to this effort is the development of unbiased lineup creation algorithms that actively address sources of bias in the data. For example, AI systems could be programmed to ensure that fillers are selected based on a more diverse set of features rather than relying solely on basic similarity metrics. Moreover, the use of explainable AI (XAI) can provide transparency in how lineup members are chosen, allowing human administrators to review and verify the fairness of the system’s output.
Human oversight remains essential in this process to ensure ethical considerations are upheld. While AI can automate many aspects of lineup creation, human intervention is critical for quality control and to prevent algorithmic biases from affecting outcomes. Evidence-based solutions are necessary to address these challenges, and together these measures can create a more robust system for eyewitness identification that balances technological innovation with human judgment and ethical responsibility.
Evidence-Based Solutions
Sequential lineups remain a fundamental method for improving eyewitness identification procedures. Unlike simultaneous lineups, where witnesses view all suspects at once, sequential lineups present suspects one at a time, encouraging absolute rather than relative judgments. This shift significantly enhances the accuracy of identifications (Wells et al., 1998).
Another key improvement is informing witnesses that the perpetrator may not be present in the lineup. This reduces the psychological pressure to make a selection, which can otherwise lead to misidentifications. Without this instruction, witnesses may feel compelled to choose the person who looks most like the perpetrator, even if the actual suspect is absent (Brewer & Wells, 2006). By alleviating this pressure, the accuracy of witness decisions improves.
Innovative practices like "ranking lineups" ask witnesses to rank lineup members based on resemblance to the perpetrator, rather than making a single, definitive choice. This approach encourages deeper cognitive processing, reducing the reliance on snap judgments. As a result, ranking lineups have been shown to improve identification accuracy and provide more data for investigators (Tuttle et al., 2025).
An additional crucial enhancement is recording a witness's confidence level immediately after identification. Witness confidence is a useful indicator of reliability, but it tends to inflate over time due to external influences like law enforcement feedback or media exposure. Capturing confidence at the moment of identification provides a more objective measure of certainty, which can be used to assess the accuracy of the witness’s memory (Wixted & Wells, 2017). This insight is particularly valuable in legal proceedings, helping jurors and judges better evaluate the reliability of testimony.
Comprehensive training for law enforcement officers in cognitive interviewing techniques also strengthens the integrity of witness identifications. Cognitive interviewing uses open-ended questions and mental context reconstruction to help witnesses recall more accurate details while minimizing suggestive influences. Officers trained in these techniques support witnesses in providing richer, more accurate descriptions of suspects, enhancing lineup reliability.
Finally, cross-cultural research is essential to ensure lineup procedures are fair for witnesses from diverse backgrounds. Memory specificity, recognition strategies, and decision-making styles vary across cultures. Wang (2021) notes that "cross-cultural differences in memory specificity" highlight the importance of culturally responsive lineup protocols.Without these adjustments, certain cultural groups may face a higher risk of misidentification.Tailoring lineup procedures to address these differences promotes fairness and reduces bias.
Conclusion
Eyewitness identification plays a pivotal role in criminal justice, but its susceptibility to error calls for the need for reform. Flawed lineup procedures have contributed to wrongful convictions, eroding trust in the system. Addressing these issues requires a shift toward evidence-based practices that prioritize accuracy, fairness, and cultural responsiveness. Sequential lineups, which encourage absolute judgments, have proven more effective than simultaneous lineups. Similarly, instructing witnesses that the perpetrator may not be present reduces the psychological pressure to choose, while ranking lineups promote deeper cognitive processing. Recording witness confidence at the time of identification offers a critical measure of reliability, avoiding inflated confidence later. Law enforcement training in cognitive interviewing further strengthens identification accuracy by supporting unbiased recall of events.
Cross-cultural research highlights the significance of tailoring lineup protocols to ensure fairness across diverse populations, mitigating cultural biases that can affect memory and recognition. Technological innovations, like AI-assisted lineup systems, present new opportunities to standardize and enhance lineup fairness, but ethical oversight remains crucial. Together, these options offer a comprehensive strategy to improve the accuracy of eyewitness identifications, protect the innocent, and fortify public confidence in the justice system.
1/27/25
Works Cited
Eyewitness Misidentification.” Department of Public Advocacy, 12 July 2024, https://dpa.ky.gov/kentucky-department-of-public-advocacy/about-dpa/kip/causes/misid/
Deffenbacher KA, Bornstein BH, Penrod SD, McGorty EK. A meta-analytic review of the effects of high stress on eyewitness memory. Law Hum Behav. 2004 Dec;28(6):687-706. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15732653/
Pickel, K. L. (1999). The influence of context on the "weapon focus" effect. Law and Human Behavior, 23(3), 299–311. https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1023%2FA:1022356431375
Wells, G. L., Small, M., Penrod, S., Malpass, R. S., Fulero, S. M., & Brimacombe, C. A. E. (1998). Eyewitness identification procedures: Recommendations for lineups and photospreads.Law and Human Behavior, 22(6), 603–647.https://psycnet.apa.org/fulltext/1998-03228-001.html
Arndt, Jason. “Misinformation Effect.” Misinformation Effect - an Overview | ScienceDirect Topics, 2012, https://www.sciencedirect.com/topics/agricultural-and-biological-sciences/misinformation-effect
Brewer, N., & Wells, G. L. (2006). The confidence-accuracy relationship in eyewitness identification: Effects of lineup instructions, foil similarity, and target-absent base rates. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 12(1), 11–30. https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2F1076-898X.12.1.11
Wells, G. L., Kovera, M. B., Douglass, A. B., Brewer, N., Meissner, C. A., & Wixted, J.T. (2020). Policy and procedure recommendations for the collection and preservation of. eyewitness identification evidence.Law and Human Behavior, 44(1), 3–36. https://psycnet.apa.org/fulltext/2020-06220-002.html
Greenspan, R. L., & Loftus, E. F. (2021). Patterns in the use of best practices for eyewitness identifications in the field. Psychology, Crime & Law, 29(2), 161–181. https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/1068316X.2021.2018436
Wang, Qi. “The Cultural Foundation of Human Memory.” Annual Review of Psychology, Annual Reviews, 4 Jan. 2021, https://www.annualreviews.org/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-psych-070920-023638.
Quigley-McBride, A.and D. Negrabee. Beyond the Description: Investigating Consistency and Accuracy in Eyewitness Descriptions of Culprits. OSF, 29 Sept. 2024. https://osf.io/bcd74/resources
Tuttle, M. D, Starns, J. J, Cohen, A. L (2025) Protecting the innocent in eyewitness identification: An analysis of simultaneous and ranking lineups, Journal of Memory and Language, Volume 140, 104581, ISSN 0749-596X. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0749596X24000846?via%3Dihub
Wixted, J. T., & Wells, G. L. (2017). The relationship between eyewitness confidence and identification accuracy: A new synthesis. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 18(1), 10–65. http://sage.cnpereading.com/paragraph/article/?doi=10.1177/1529100616686966